Parieto-occipital sulcus

Only a small part of the parieto-occipital sulcus, or parietooccipital fissure is seen on the lateral surface of the hemisphere, its chief part being on the medial surface.

Parieto-occipital sulcus
Fig. 726: Lateral surface of left cerebral hemisphere, viewed from the side.
Fig. 727: Medial surface of left cerebral hemisphere.
Details
Identifiers
Latinsulcus parietooccipitalis, fissura parietooccipitalis
NeuroNames52
NeuroLex IDbirnlex_1428
TAA14.1.09.108
FMA83754
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy

The lateral part of the parieto-occipital sulcus (Fig. 726) is situated about 5 centimeters (cm) in front of the occipital pole of the hemisphere, and measures about 1.25 cm. in length.

The medial part of the parieto-occipital sulcus (Fig. 727) runs downward and forward as a deep cleft on the medial surface of the hemisphere, and joins the calcarine fissure below and behind the posterior end of the corpus callosum. In most cases it contains a submerged gyrus. The parieto-occipital sulcus marks the boundary between the cuneus and precuneus, and also between the parietal and occipital lobes.

Function

The parieto-occipital lobe has been found in various neuroimaging studies, including PET (positron-emission-tomography) studies,[1][2][3][4] and SPECT (single-photon emission computed tomography) studies,[5][6] to be involved along with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during planning.

References

This article incorporates text in the public domain from page 820 of the 20th edition of Gray's Anatomy (1918)

  1. Owen, Adrian M.; Doyon, Julien; Petrides, Michael; Evans, Alan C. (1996). "Planning and Spatial Working Memory: a Positron Emission Tomography Study in Humans". European Journal of Neuroscience. 8 (2): 353–364. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.1996.tb01219.x.
  2. Baker, S.C.; Rogers, R.D.; Owen, A.M.; Frith, C.D.; Dolan, R.J.; Frackowiak, R.S.J.; Robbins, T.W. (June 1996). "Neural Systems Engaged by Planning: a PET Study of the Tower of London Task" (PDF). Neuropsychologia. 34 (6): 515–526. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(95)00133-6. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-A39D-6. Retrieved 18 June 2014.
  3. Dagher, Alain; Owen, Adrian M.; Boecker, Henning; Brooks, David J. (October 1999). "Mapping the Network for Planning: a Correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London Task". Brain. 122 (10): 1973–1987. doi:10.1093/brain/122.10.1973. Retrieved 18 June 2014.
  4. Rowe, J.B.; Owen, Adrian M.; Johnsrude, Ingrid S.; Passingham, R.E. (2001). "Imaging the Mental Components of a Planning Task" (PDF). Neuropsychologia. 39 (3): 315–327. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00109-3. Retrieved 18 June 2014.
  5. Rezai, Karim; Andreasen, Nancy C.; Alliger, Randy; Cohen, Gregg; Swayze, Victor II; O'Leary, Daniel S. (June 1993). "The Neuropsychology of the Prefrontal Cortex". Archives of Neurology. 50 (6): 636–642. doi:10.1001/archneur.1993.00540060066020. Retrieved 18 June 2014.
  6. Morris, R.G.; Ahmed, S.; Syed, G.M.; Toone, B.K. (December 1993). "Neural Correlates of Planning Ability: Frontal Lobe Activation during the Tower of London Test". Neuropsychologia. 31 (12): 1367–1378. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(93)90104-8. PMID 8127433.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.