Skip Navigation Links
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
 CDC Home Search Health Topics A-Z

Preventing Chronic Disease: Public Health Research, Practice and Policy

View Current Issue
Issue Archive
Archivo de números en español








Emerging Infectious Diseases Journal
MMWR


 Home 

Volume 4: No. 1, January 2007

ORIGINAL RESEARCH
Case Management Protocol and Declining Blood Lead Concentrations Among Children


TABLE OF CONTENTS


Translation available Este resumen en español
  Ce résumé est en français
  本摘要中文版
  本摘要中文版
Print this article Print this article
E-mail this article E-mail this article:



Send feedback to editors Send feedback to editors
Download this article as a PDF Download this article as a PDF (364K)

You will need Adobe Acrobat Reader to view PDF files.


Navigate This Article
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
Acknowledgments
Author Information
References
Tables


Nedra S. Whitehead, PhD, MS, Richard Leiker, MS

Suggested citation for this article: Whitehead, NS, Leiker, R. Case management protocol and declining blood lead concentrations among children. Prev Chronic Dis [serial online] 2007 Jan [date cited]. Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/pcd/issues/2007/
jan/06_0023.htm
.

PEER REVIEWED

Abstract

Introduction 
Blood lead concentrations among children aged 6 years and younger become a concern at 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) or higher. The authors’ objective was to determine whether initial blood lead concentrations of 10–19 µg/dL (0.48–0.96 μmol/L) declined among children aged 3 years and younger and whether the magnitude of decline was associated with the case management protocol of the state or local childhood lead poisoning prevention program.

Methods 
The authors analyzed childhood blood lead surveillance data from 1994 through 1995 and case management protocols from six states that reported the results of all blood lead tests. The study included 2109 children aged 2 years or younger who had a venous blood lead concentration of 10–19 µg/dL (0.48–0.96 μmol/L) and a follow-up venous blood lead test within 3 to 12 months.

Results 
Overall, blood lead concentrations increased by 0.25 µg/dL (0.01 μmol/L) between the time of the initial elevated blood lead test and the follow-up test, but concentrations declined by 1.96 µg/dL (0.09 μmol/L) among children covered by a case management protocol that included a home visit and by 0.92 µg/dL (0.04 μmol/L) among those covered by a protocol that included a lead source investigation. The decline remained significant after we adjusted for the child’s age.

Conclusion 
These findings suggest that childhood lead prevention programs should consider focusing their efforts on home visits and lead source investigations.

Back to top

Introduction

Children are exposed to lead from multiple sources, including lead-based paint and lead-contaminated dust. This exposure can have chronic consequences. Preschool children with blood lead concentrations greater than 9 µg/dL (0.43 μmol/L) have lower intelligence and more performance problems on average than do children who are unexposed to lead (1,2). Research by Bellinger et al (1991) and Ruff et al (1996) suggests that scores for children aged 2 years on measures of cognitive performance drop one point for every 1-µg/dL (0.05-μmol/L) increase in blood lead concentration (1,2). Although the decreases in cognitive performance are small individually, in the aggregate they may result in more children with behavioral problems and lower intelligence. Childhood lead exposure can also result in adult chronic health problems (e.g., adverse pregnancy outcomes [3,4], hypertension [5,6]).

In 1991, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) designated blood lead concentrations of 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) or higher as the level of concern for children aged 6 years and younger. Children were not considered to need environmental or medical intervention unless their blood lead concentration was 20 µg/dL (0.97 μmol/L) or higher (7). An estimated 890,000 children aged 6 years and younger had blood lead concentrations of 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) or greater in the United States from 1991 through 1994 (8). Although blood lead concentrations among young children have declined since 1994, exposure to lead continues to be a significant problem (9).

Blood lead concentrations of exposed, untreated children increase until children are about aged 2 years and decline thereafter (10). Behavioral and environmental changes to reduce lead exposure are necessary to lower blood lead concentrations before children are aged 2 years. Lead accumulates in bone during chronic exposure and may be released when bone is reabsorbed during pregnancy or lactation (11,12). This release increases maternal blood lead concentrations associated with fetal neurological damage (13,14).

Childhood lead poisoning prevention programs have various case management protocols for children with blood lead concentrations of 10–19 µg/dL (0.48–0.96 μmol/L), but the effect of these case management protocols on children’s blood lead concentrations is unknown. We analyzed childhood blood lead surveillance data from six states to examine changes in blood lead concentrations among children aged 2 years and younger to determine whether there was any relationship with case management protocol. We examined the following questions:

  • Does blood lead concentration decline after an initial venous blood lead test result of 10–19 µg/dL (0.48–0.96 μmol/L)?
  • If so, is the size of the decline associated with state or local case management protocol?
  • Does the effect of the case management protocol differ if the initial blood lead concentration is between 10–14 µg/dL (0.48–0.71 μmol/L) or between 15–19 µg/dL (0.72–0.96 μmol/L)?
  • Does the effect of case management protocol remain after controlling for a child’s demographic characteristics?

Back to top

Methods

We defined a case of borderline elevated blood lead concentration as a venous blood lead concentration of 10–19 µg/dL (0.48–0.96 μmol/L), regardless of the case definition used by state and local lead poisoning prevention programs. The case management protocol for a state or county was defined as the method of contact required under the protocol (i.e., mail, telephone, or home visit) and the type of service to be delivered under the protocol (i.e., educational materials on lead exposure prevention alone or lead source investigation) for children with a given blood lead concentration. Each child with blood lead concentrations of 10–19 µg/dL (0.48–0.96 μmol/L) was assigned a method of contact and a type of service according to information on case management protocol provided by the coordinator of each state lead poisoning prevention program. One state, Wisconsin, provided county-level information. Children were assumed to have received the services called for under the case management protocol of their state or county of residence.

Children’s demographic information and blood lead test data came from CDC’s childhood blood lead database, which is compiled from state childhood blood lead surveillance data (15). Test results are submitted by laboratories and physicians to state surveillance programs, which link the laboratory tests of each child to ensure that duplicate results are deleted and sequential tests are accurately identified. We limited our analysis to six states that 1) had laws requiring that the results of all blood lead tests on children be reported to the state health department, 2) submitted blood lead surveillance data from 1994 and 1995 to CDC, and 3) agreed to have their data used for our analysis. These states were Iowa, Montana, New Mexico, Ohio (only tests performed after April 1995, when universal reporting began, were included), Rhode Island, and Wisconsin.  

In 1996, guidelines for blood lead screening changed. Under the new guidelines, targeted screening of children in high risk areas and populations was recommended instead of universal screening — a change that altered the population of children who were tested. We limited our analysis to children who had an initial venous blood lead test concentration of 10–19 µg/dL (0.48–0.96 μmol/L) before they were aged 2 years and who had at least one follow-up venous blood test 3 to 12 months after their initial test. These parameters were selected to ensure sufficient time for interventions to affect blood lead concentrations, to examine the long-term effects of the interventions, and to allow for varied case management protocols. Although a follow-up test within this period was recommended for all children with a venous blood lead concentration of 10–19 µg/dL (0.48–0.96 μmol/L), the timing of the follow-up test varied by case management protocol. The test could be either a venous or a capillary test (7). We limited our analysis to venous tests because greater variability in capillary blood test results made it difficult to identify small changes in blood lead levels. Among the six states, 121,862 children had a blood lead test result in the database, and 4606 (3.8%) children had an initial elevated level between 10–19 µg/dL (0.48–0.96 μmol/L) and at least one follow-up test. Of these children, 2109 (46%) met our study criteria. Our data set included all available test results as well as demographic and intervention data for children who met our study criteria.

We computed the change in blood lead concentration for a child as the difference between the blood lead concentration at the first elevated venous test and the concentration at the first venous follow-up test completed 3 to 12 months after the initial elevated test. We calculated mean changes as the average of individual changes for the group and used paired t tests to test for significant changes. We stratified our analysis by blood lead concentration because we expected the magnitude of any decline in blood lead concentration to be related to the initial concentration.

We used Kruskal-Wallis tests to compare mean changes in blood lead concentrations, analysis of variance (ANOVA) to compare the mean number of months needed for blood lead concentrations to decline to less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L), and the Mantel-Haenszel chi-square test to determine differences in the proportion of children whose blood lead concentration was less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) at the end of follow-up. We examined the relationship between case management protocol and changes in blood lead concentration over the entire follow-up period and controlled for the age of the child with generalized linear modeling. Quadratic splines with knots at 20, 50, and 80 percentiles for each variable allowed the relationship between a child’s age at the initial test, or during the time between tests, and blood lead concentration to differ for different ages or time spans. All the venous blood test results available for a child were used in generalized equalizing equation modeling with autoregressive correlation to examine the longitudinal relationship between case management protocol and blood lead concentration. We controlled for age at the initial and follow-up tests. We used SAS Version 8 (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC) for all analyses (16).

Back to top

Results

The children in this study were served by childhood lead poisoning prevention programs that provided parental education by mail (78%), telephone (21%), or home visits (<1%) when a child’s blood lead concentration was 10–14 µg/dL (0.48–0.71 μmol/L). Forty-eight percent of children in the study were served by childhood lead poisoning prevention programs that also provided parental education by mail when a child’s blood lead concentration was 15–19 µg/dL (0.72–0.96 μmol/L). The other 52% were covered by childhood lead poisoning prevention programs that provided home visits. Eighty-four percent of the children were covered by programs that provided education alone and 16% by programs that provided investigations of the source of lead exposure (Table 1).

Forty percent of children in the study were aged 13 months and younger when they had their first elevated blood lead test result (Table 1). A public agency paid for the initial blood lead test of 33% of the children; 11% had private payers, and payment source was unknown for 56%. Race and ethnicity were known for 68% of the children. Of all children in the study, 29% of the children were white, 21% were black, and 18% were Hispanic. Approximately one half of the children were from Wisconsin.  

On average, blood lead concentrations increased by 0.25 µg/dL (0.01 μmol/L) between the first elevated venous blood lead test and the first follow-up test done 3 to 12 months after the initial elevated test. The direction and magnitude of change in blood lead concentration varied by the child’s age at the initial elevated test, whether a public or private entity paid for the test, the child’s race or ethnicity, and state of residence. Blood lead levels declined, on average, among older children, those whose test was paid for by private funds, those who were not white or Hispanic, and residents of states other than Ohio and Wisconsin. The sex of the child was not associated with changes in blood lead concentrations (Table 1).

Overall, blood lead concentrations declined most among children whose case management protocol called for a home visit (Table 2). Blood lead concentrations decreased by 1.96 µg/dL (0.09 μmol/L) in these children and by 0.72 µg/dL (0.03 μmol/L) among children in families receiving a telephone call as follow-up. In contrast, blood lead concentrations increased 1.18 µg/dL (0.06 μmol/L) among children receiving mail follow-up. Table 3 shows that blood lead concentrations declined by 0.92 µg/dL (0.04 μmol/L), or 6%, on average among children covered by a protocol that included a lead source investigation but rose by 0.36 µg/dL (0.02 μmol/L), or 4%, among children covered by protocols that provided only mailed educational materials.

The contrast between protocols was even more striking for children whose initial blood lead concentration was 10–14 µg/dL (0.48–0.71 μmol/L). Table 2 shows that lead concentrations rose by 1.49 µg/dL (0.07 μmol/L), 13.5%, among children whose parents received mailed materials but declined by 2.20 µg/dL (0.11 μmol/L), 18.8%, among those who received a home visit. Also among children with initial blood lead concentration of 10–14 µg/dL, lead source investigation protocols were associated with an average decline of 1.76 µg/dL (0.08 μmol/L) in lead concentration, whereas education-only protocols were associated with an average increase of 1.07 µg/dL (0.05 μmol/L) (Table 3).

The association with type of contact was less marked for children whose initial blood lead concentration was 15–19 µg/dL (0.72–0.96 μmol/L), and there was no overall difference between blood lead concentrations among children receiving mailed educational materials and those receiving a lead source investigation (Table 3). Blood lead concentrations increased by 0.48 µg/dL (0.02 μmol/L) among children who received mailed materials but declined by 1.95 µg/dL (0.09 μmol/L) among those who received home visits (Table 2). Blood lead concentrations declined by 0.79 µg/dL (0.04 μmol/L) among children who received mailed educational materials only and by 0.72 µg/dL (0.03 μmol/L) among those who received a lead source investigation (Table 3). The association between type of case management protocol and changes in blood lead concentration persisted even after we controlled for a child’s age at the first elevated test result and follow-up tests (Tables 2,3). Blood lead concentrations remained lower at the time of later tests among children who received telephone contact or home visits than among those contacted by mail (Table 2). Concentrations also remained lower among those who received a lead source investigation than among those who received mailed educational materials only (Table 3). The results were similar when the analysis was limited to children aged 15 months or younger at the time of their initial blood lead test (data not shown).

Among children with initial blood lead concentrations of 15–19 µg/dL (0.72–0.96 μmol/L) who received a home visit, blood lead concentrations of those who received mailed educational materials declined by 2.49 µg/dL (0.12 μmol/L), a larger decline than that of those who received lead source investigations 0.72 µg/dL (0.03 μmol/L) (data not shown). The difference remained after we adjusted for a child’s age but was not significant at later tests.

We did not control for race, ethnicity, and payment source in these models because of missing data; only 23% of records included information on these variables. When we fit models including race and payment source, we found that home visits and lead source investigations among children with initial blood lead concentrations of 10–14 µg/dL (0.48–0.71 μmol/L) were still associated with a significant decline in blood lead concentrations between the time of initial test and first follow-up test, but telephone contact was not significant (data not shown). Including these variables did not change any longitudinal effects significantly.

Blood lead concentrations declined to less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) by the last reported test among 43% of children whose initial blood lead concentration was 10–14 µg/dL (0.48–0.71 μmol/L) and among 23% of children whose initial blood lead concentration was 15–19 µg/dL (0.72–0.96 μmol/L) (Table 4). On average, the time required for blood lead concentrations to drop to less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) was 12 months for children with a concentration of 10–14 µg/dL (0.48–0.71 μmol/L) and 13 months for those with an initial concentration of 15–19 µg/dL (0.72–0.96 μmol/L). Home visits were associated with the highest proportion of children with blood lead concentrations of less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) by the last reported follow-up test and with the shortest average time required for blood lead concentrations to decline to less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L).

Back to top

Discussion

We found that home visit protocols were associated with a larger decline in blood lead concentrations than mail or telephone contact protocols, regardless of a child’s initial blood lead concentration. Mailed educational materials alone were not associated with lower blood lead concentrations.

There are several possible limitations to our study. One is that we may have underestimated the effects of case management protocol. Another is that tested children may not have actually received the services called for by the lead poisoning prevention program in their area. We also had no information on the details of interventions and how these may differ among programs. State laws require the reporting of blood lead tests, but some results, especially those below 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) or conducted by out-of-state laboratories, may not have been reported or linked to an appropriate child. We used the first venous test performed at least 3 months after the index test of 10–19 μg/dL, but we included children who had at least one follow-up test up to 12 months. Blood lead concentrations among children who had more time between the initial test and the follow-up test would have more time to decline, and this effect could bias our results. The analyses that controlled for the timing of tests, however, should not be affected by this limitation.

The effect of other limitations is less clear. We had no information on many variables, such as iron or calcium intake, that affect blood lead concentrations. Demographic characteristics were missing for many children. We cannot determine how results may have differed if we had been able to control for race and payment source, but results of analyses that included these variables were similar to ones that did not. For these factors to affect our results, however, they would have to be associated with both the case management protocol of the state or county childhood lead poisoning prevention program and with changes in a child’s blood lead concentration. Although many factors are associated with an initial elevated blood lead concentration, few have been found to affect changes in blood lead concentration (16-20). Our study was limited to states that require all blood lead test results be reported to the state health department and may not apply to states not included in the analysis.

Surveillance data result from tests performed at the request of a child’s parents or physician, and circumstances for these children may differ from circumstances of children who are not tested. This limitation may affect the applicability of our findings to other children. Parents of children who had an elevated blood lead test result but who did not take their children for follow-up tests may be less likely to implement measures to control lead exposure than parents of children who did receive follow-up tests. If this is true, we would expect all case management protocols to be less effective in children overall than we found among children in our study.

The total amount of lead stored in tissues in a child’s body can affect blood lead concentrations and obscure the efficacy of interventions. Lead is released during bone turnover until all the stored lead has been released from tissues. Rust et al estimated that among children aged 2 years and younger, bone lead stores could elevate blood lead concentrations for up to 1 year after all sources of lead exposure were removed (21). Bone lead stores could not be estimated for children in this study and may have caused us to underestimate the efficacy of all case management protocols. Differences in bone lead stores, however, could account for our results only if children who received mail or education interventions had higher bone lead stores on average than those of the same age and initial blood lead concentration who received home visits or lead source investigations.

Our study has several strengths in relation to other studies of interventions to reduce blood lead concentrations. We had a large sample, which allowed us to detect small effects on blood lead concentrations, and a diverse study population in terms of race and ethnicity, population density, and geographic region. Our blood lead tests were linked by child and allowed us to compare changes in an individual’s blood lead concentration instead of an average for a group.

We identified five randomized trial studies that examined the effectiveness of interventions to reduce blood lead concentrations among children who had concentrations of less than 20 µg/dL (0.97 μmol/L) (17,18,22,23). These interventions included professional house cleaning (17,22), provision of household cleaning supplies and instructions (23), vacuuming with high efficiency particulate (HEPA) air filters (18), and provision of an individualized care plan that included multiple home visits and parental teaching on lead exposure prevention (24). Only one study intervention was effective: repeated professional house cleaning resulted in a 2.1 µg/dL (0.1 μmol/L) decrease in blood lead concentration (22). In four of the studies, however, the controls received a home visit that included information about prevention of lead poisoning or identification of lead hazards; this information was likely similar to information provided by lead poisoning prevention programs (17,18,22,24). The changes in mean blood lead concentration among these control groups varied after 3 months: −5.9 µg/dL (–0.28 μmol/L) (17); −1.0 µg/dL (−0.05 μmol/L) (18), +0.1 µg/dL (<0.01 μmol/L) (22), and −6.2 µg/dL (−0.3 μmol/L) (24). Rhoads et al included information on accident prevention and provided safety equipment in addition to information on lead hazards and lead poisoning prevention. The presence of safety and accident information may have lessened the impact of the lead poisoning prevention information (22). Hilts et al conducted their study near an active lead smelter; this location increased the likelihood of lead exposure. Aschengrau et al had only 24 children in randomized groups and included older children whose blood lead levels may already have begun to decline. The comparison group in the study by Brown et al received one or two educational home visits. In the remaining study, controls received a brochure on lead poisoning prevention (23). The blood lead concentrations of these children declined slightly [0.60 µg/dL (0.03 μmol/L)], but the average age at recruitment was 20 months, so the decline may be attributable to the natural decline as children age.

All of these other studies measured the change in group mean blood lead concentrations. We measured the change in blood lead concentrations for individual children. The decline in blood lead concentrations among children in our study who received home visits [−1.96 µg/dL (0.09 μmol/L)], however, was within the range of changes in blood lead concentrations among these control groups.

Schultz et al, in a study of children followed by a local lead poisoning prevention program, examined the average changes in blood lead concentrations before and after the program implemented home education visits among children who had initial blood lead concentrations of 20–24 µg/dL (0.97–1.16 μmol/L) (25). These authors found that blood lead concentrations declined by 3.9 µg/dL (0.19 μmol/L) on average among children who received home visits but increased by 1.0 µg/dL (0.05 μmol/L) among children who received no follow-up. They may have found a larger decline in blood lead concentrations than we did because the children in their study had higher initial blood lead concentrations than those in our study.

The decline in blood lead concentrations among children covered by a case management protocol that included a home visit was larger and faster than predicted by Neimuth and Schultz for children not receiving interventions (26). For children with the age and initial blood lead concentration ranges of this study, Niemuth and Schultz predicted changes at 12-month follow-up tests that ranged from a 7% increase for children aged 6 months with an initial blood lead concentration of 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) to an 8.2% decrease for children aged 24 months with an initial blood lead concentration of 15 µg/dL (0.72 μmol/L) (26). We found a 19% decline among children whose initial blood lead concentration was 10–14 µg/dL (0.48–0.71 μmol/L) and who were covered by a case management protocol that included a home visit and a 12% decline among those whose initial blood lead concentration was 15–19 µg/dL (0.72–0.96 μmol/L). Mailed educational materials were associated with a 10% increase in blood lead concentrations; this increase was the same or higher than predicted for children receiving no treatment.

Roberts et al estimated the time that would be required for blood lead concentrations to decline to less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) among children who received no intervention. In our study, the time required for initial blood lead concentrations of 10–14 µg/dL (0.48–0.71 μmol/L) to decline to less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) among children covered by home visit protocols was similar to that estimated by Roberts et al for children with blood lead concentrations in this range who received no intervention (27). Among children whose initial blood lead concentration was 15–19 µg/dL (0.72–0.96 μmol/L) and who received a home visit, however, blood lead concentrations declined to less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) in 10 months — 4 months faster than was estimated by Roberts et al (27).

Some clinicians have questioned the value of following children with blood lead concentrations of less than 15 µg/dL (0.72 μmol/L) (28). Although a systematic evaluation of the effect of different types of programs is needed, our results suggest that blood lead concentrations of less than 20 µg/dL (0.97 μmol/L) can be significantly reduced with protocols that include home visits and lead source investigations. This reduction is valuable because recent research has suggested that even children with blood lead concentrations of less than 10 µg/dL (0.48 μmol/L) have identifiable cognitive deficits associated with lead exposure because stored lead has long-term effects (16,29). Other research has found that strong state policies requiring lead abatement in housing result in fewer children exposed to lead and fewer children with elevated blood lead concentrations (30,31). Home investigations for children with borderline elevated blood lead concentrations, combined with strong lead abatement policies, could lower current and future lead exposure among children.

Back to top

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr Hoyt Wilson, Division of Reproductive Health, CDC, for his statistical assistance. Mr. Leiker receives salary support from CDC Cooperative Agreement US7/CCU-022875.

Back to top

Author Information

Corresponding Author: Nedra S. Whitehead, Research Triangle Institute (RTI) International, Social and Statistical Sciences, 2951 Flowers Rd, Suite 119, Atlanta, GA 30341. Telephone: 770-986-5051. E-mail: nwhitehead@rti.org.

Author Affiliations: Richard Leiker, Oregon Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention Program, Environmental and Occupational Epidemiology Section, Oregon Health Services, Office of Disease Prevention and Epidemiology, Portland, Ore.

Back to top

References

  1. Bellinger D, Sloman J, Leviton A, Rabinowitz M, Needleman HL, Waternaux C. Low-level lead exposure and children's cognitive function in the preschool years. [Published erratum in: Pediatrics 1994;93(2):A28]. Pediatrics 1991;87(2):219-27.
  2. Ruff HA, Markowitz ME, Bijur PE, Rosen JF. Relationships among blood lead levels, iron deficiency, and cognitive development in two-year-old children. Environ Health Perspect 1996;104(2):180-5.
  3. Hernandez-Avila M, Peterson KE, Gonzalez-Cossio T, Sanin LH, Aro A, Schnaas L, et al. Effect of maternal bone lead on length and head circumference of newborns and 1-month-old infants. Arch Environ Health 2002;57(5):482-8.
  4. Rothenberg SJ, Kondrashov V, Manalo M, Jiang J, Cuellar R, Garcia M, et al. Increases in hypertension and blood pressure during pregnancy with increased bone lead levels. Am J Epidemiol 2002;156(12):1079-87.
  5. Glenn BS, Stewart WF, Links JM, Todd AC, Schwartz BS. The longitudinal association of lead with blood pressure. Epidemiology 2003;14(1):30-6.
  6. Gerr F, Letz R, Stokes L, Chettle D, McNeill F, Kaye W. Association between bone lead concentration and blood pressure among young adults. Am J Ind Med 2002;42(2):98-106.
  7. Roper WL, Houk VN, Falk H, Binder S. Preventing lead poisoning in young children: a statement by the Centers for Disease Control--October 1991. Atlanta (GA): Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 1991.
  8. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Update: blood lead levels--United States, 1991-1994. [Published erratum in: MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 1997;46(26):607]. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 1997;46(7):141-6.
  9. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Blood lead levels in young children--United States and selected states, 1996-1999. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 2000;49(50):1133-7.
  10. Dietrich KN, Succop PA, Berger OG, Hammond PB, Bornschein RL. Lead exposure and the cognitive development of urban preschool children: the Cincinnati Lead Study cohort at age 4 years. Neurotoxicol Teratol 1991;13(2):203-11.
  11. Gulson BL, Jameson CW, Mahaffey KR, Mizon KJ, Korsch MJ, Vimpani G. Pregnancy increases mobilization of lead from maternal skeleton. J Lab Clin Med 1997;130(1):51-62.
  12. Gulson BL, Mahaffey KR, Jameson CW, Mizon KJ, Korsch MJ, Cameron MA, et al. Mobilization of lead from the skeleton during the postnatal period is larger than during pregnancy. J Lab Clin Med 1998;131(4):324-9
  13. Emory E, Ansari Z, Pattillo R, Archibold E, Chevalier J. Maternal blood lead effects on infant intelligence at age 7 months. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2003;188(4):S26-32.
  14. Gonzalez-Cossio T, Peterson KE, Sanin LH, Fishbein E, Palazuelos E, Aro A, et al. Decrease in birth weight in relation to maternal bone-lead burden. Pediatrics 1997;100(5):856-62.
  15. National Childhood Blood Lead Surveillance Database. Atlanta (GA): Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Environmental Health;[cited 2006 Oct 1]. Available from:      http://www.cdc.gov/nceh/lead/surv/database/database.htm
  16. SAS Institute, Inc. SAS Language Reference, Version 6. 1st Ed. Cary (NC): SAS Institute, Inc; 1990.
  17. Lanphear BP, Dietrich K, Auinger P, Cox C. Cognitive deficits associated with blood lead concentrations <10 microg/dL in US children and adolescents. Public Health Rep 2000;115(6):521-9.
  18. Aschengrau A, Hardy S, Mackey P, Pultinas D. The impact of low technology lead hazard reduction activities among children with mildly elevated blood lead levels. Environ Res 1998;79(1):41-50.
  19. Hilts SR, Hertzman C, Marion SA. A controlled trial of the effect of HEPA vacuuming on childhood lead exposure. Can J Public Health 1995;86(5):345-50.
  20. Jordan CM, Yust BL, Robison LL, Hannan P, Deinard AS. A randomized trial of education to prevent lead burden in children at high risk for lead exposure: efficacy as measured by blood lead monitoring. Environ Health Perspect 2003;111(16):1947-51.
  21. Staes C, Matte T, Copley CG, Flanders D, Binder S. Retrospective study of the impact of lead-based paint hazard remediation on children's blood lead levels in St. Louis, Missouri. Am J Epidemiol 1994;139(10):1016-26.
  22. Rust SW, Kumar P, Burgoon DA, Niemuth NA, Schultz BD.  Influence of bone-lead stores on the observed effectiveness of lead hazard intervention. Environ Res 1999;81(3):175-84.
  23. Rhoads GG, Ettinger AS, Weisel CP, Buckley TJ, Goldman KD, Adgate J, et al. The effect of dust lead control on blood lead in toddlers: a randomized trial. Pediatrics 1999;103(3):551-5.
  24. Lanphear BP, Winter NL, Apetz L, Eberly S, Weitzman M. A randomized trial of the effect of dust control on children's blood lead levels. Pediatrics 1996;98(1):35-40.
  25. Brown MJ, McLaine P, Dixon S, Simon P. A randomized, community-based trial of home visiting to reduce blood lead levels in children. Pediatrics 2006;117(1):147-53.
  26. Schultz B, Pawel D, Murphy A. A retrospective examination of in-home educational visits to reduce childhood lead levels. Environ Res 1999;80(4):364-8.
  27. Niemuth NA, Wood BJ, Schultz BD. Estimated change in blood lead concentration in control populations. Arch Environ Health 2001; 56(6):542-51.
  28. Roberts JR, Reigart JR, Ebeling M, Hulsey TC. Time required for blood lead levels to decline in nonchelated children. J Toxicol Clin Toxicol 2001;39(2):153-60.
  29. Block B, Szekely K, Escobar M. Difficulties in evaluating abnormal lead screening results in children. J Am Board Fam Pract 1996;9(6):405-10.
  30. Canfield RL, Henderson CR Jr, Cory-Slechta DA, Cox C, Jusko TA, Lanphear BP. Intellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 microg per deciliter. N Engl J Med 2003;348(16):1517-26.
  31. Sargent JD, Dalton M, Demidenko E, Simon P, Klein RZ. The association between state housing policy and lead poisoning in children. Am J Public Health 1999;89(11):1690-5.
  32. Brown MJ, Gardner J, Sargent JD, Swartz K, Hu H, Timperi R. The effectiveness of housing policies in reducing children's lead exposure. Am J Public Health 2001;91(4):621-4.

Back to top

 



Tables

Return to your place in the textTable 1. Changes in Blood Lead Concentrations Between Initial Blood Lead Test and Follow-up Test by Demographic Characteristics Among Children Aged 2 Years and Younger in Six U.S. States, 1994–1995
Characteristics No. Children (%) Mean Changea
(μg/dL) (SE)
t Test
P Value
Kruskal-Wallis
P Value
Total 2109 (100.0) 0.25 (0.2) .11
Age at first test, mo.
0-7 58 (2.8) 2.40 (1.1) .04 .005
8-10 246 (11.7) 1.34 (0.5) .009
11-13 540 (25.6) 0.53 (0.3) .12
14-16 431 (20.4) 0.14 (0.3) .67
17-19 395 (18.7) −0.39 (0.4) .27
20-24 439 (20.8) −0.29 (0.3) .38
Sex
Male 1054 (50.0) 0.32 (0.2) .16 .67
Female 937 (44.4) 0.16 (0.2) .51
Unknown 118 (5.6) 0.46 (0.7) .49
Payment source for laboratory test
Public 697 (33.0) 0.02 (0.3) .94 <.001
Private 225 (10.7) −1.31 (0.5) .005
Unknown 1187 (56.3) 0.69 (0.2) .001
Race and ethnicity
White 610 (28.9) 0.83 (0.3) .007 .02
Black 434 (20.6) −0.10 (0.3) .74
Native American 15 (0.7) −1.53 (1.7) .38
Hispanic 385 (18.3) 0.50 (0.4) .18
Other and unknown 665 (31.5) −0.14 (0.3) .65
State
Iowa 393 (18.6) 1.41 (0.3) <.001 <.001
Montana 33 (1.6) 3.03 (0.7) <.001
New Mexico 16 (0.8) 3.13 (1.2) .02
Ohio 224 (10.6) 0.34 (0.4) .43
Rhode Island 423 (20.0) 1.09 (0.4) .004
Wisconsin 1020 (48.4) 1.6 (0.2) <.001

aBlood lead concentrations are expressed in µg/dL. Conversion to SI µmol/L units is µg/dL × 0.0483 = µmol/L.

Return to your place in the textTable 2. Unadjusted and Adjusted Mean Changes in Blood Lead Concentration by Initial Level and Method of Family Contact Among Children Aged 2 Years and Younger in Six U.S. States,a 1994–1995

Method of Contact for All
Children in Study

No.  Children Mean Change
Unadjusted for Ageb
Mean Change Adjusted for Agec Mean Decline Adjusted for Aged
µg/dLe (SE) t Test P Value µg/dL (SE) Wald P Value µg/dL (SE) Wald P Value
Children with initial blood lead concentration
10-19 µg/dL
2109 0.25 (0.2) .11
Mail 1383 1.18 (0.2) <.001 Ref Ref
Telephone 262 −0.72 (0.02) .04 −1.95 (0.5) <.001 −0.80 (0.3) .01
Home visit 464 −1.96 (0.4) <.001 −2.98 (0.4) <.001 −2.00 (0.2) <.001
Children with initial blood lead concentration 
10-14 µg/dL
1233 0.99 (0.2) <.001
Mail 961 1.49 (0.2) <.001 Ref Ref
Telephone 262 −0.72 (0.3) .04 −2.10 (0.5) <.001 −1.04 (0.3) <.001
Home 10 −2.20 (1.6) .20 −3.68 (2.2) .09 −0.86 (0.4) .04
Initial blood lead concentration 15-19 µg/dL 876 −0.78 (0.3) .003
Mail 422 0.48 (0.4) .20 Ref Ref
Telephone 0
Home 454 −1.95 (0.4) <.001 −2.50 (0.6) <.001 −1.87 (0.3) <.001

Ref indicates reference group.
aStates are Iowa, Montana, New Mexico, Ohio, Rhode Island, and Wisconsin.
bMean change in blood lead concentration between initial test and final follow-up test 3–12 months after initial test.
cMean change in blood lead concentration between initial test and first follow-up test 3–12 months after initial test, adjusted for child’s age.
dMean change in blood lead concentration over entire follow-up period, adjusted for child’s age at initial and follow-up testing.
eBlood lead concentrations are expressed in µg/dL. Conversion to SI µmol/L units is µg/dL × 0.0483 = µmol/L.

Return to your place in the textTable 3. Unadjusted and Adjusted Mean Changes in Blood Lead Concentration by Case Management Service Among Children Aged 2 Years and Younger in Six U.S. States,a 1994–1995

Type of  Service
for All Children in
Survey

No. Children Mean Change
Unadjusted for Ageb
Mean Change Adjusted for Agec Mean Decline Adjusted for Aged
µg/dLe (SE) t Test P Value µg/dLe (SE) Wald P Value µg/dLe (SE) Wald P Value
Initial blood lead concentration 10-19 µg/dL
Education 1939 0.36 (0.2) .03 Ref Ref
Lead source investigation 170 −0.92 (0.5) .06 −0.93 (0.6) .12 −0.89 (0.3) .001
Initial blood lead concentration 10-14 µg/dL
Education 1200 1.07 (0.2) <.001 Ref Ref
Lead source investigation 33 −1.76 (0.7) .01 −2.59 (1.2) .03 −1.81 (0.4) <.001
Initial blood lead concentration 15-19 µg/dL
Education 739 −0.79 (0.3) .007 Ref Ref
Lead source investigation 137 −0.72 (0.6) .22 0.24 (0.8) .75 −0.17 (0.3) .61

Ref indicates reference group.
aStates are Iowa, Montana, New Mexico, Ohio, Rhode Island, and Wisconsin.
bMean change in blood lead concentration between initial test and final follow-up test 3–12 months after initial test.
cMean change in blood lead concentration between initial test and end of follow-up test period 3–12 months after initial test, adjusted for child’s age.
dMean change in blood lead concentration between initial test and end of follow-up, adjusted for child’s age at initial test.
eBlood lead concentrations are expressed in µg/dL. Conversion to SI µmol/L units is µg/dL × 0.0483 = µmol/L. 

Return to your place in the textTable 4. Mean Number of Months Required to Reach Blood Lead Concentrations <10 µg/dL by End of Follow-up Period in Six States,a 1994–1995
 Protocol No. Children
Who Reached
<10 μg/dL
% Children Who Reached <10 μg/dL  (SE) Mean No.
Months to reach <10 µg/dL (SE)
χ2
P
Value
ANOVA
P
Value
Initial blood lead
concentration
10-14 μg/dL (N = 1233)
530 43.0 (1.4) 11.6 (9.9)  —  —
Method of contact
  Mail 353 36.7 (1.6) 12.3 (0.6) <.001 .06
  Telephone 169 64.5 (3.0) 10.3 (0.6)
  Home visit 8 80 (13.3) 8.9 (1.7)
Case management service
  Education 505 42.1 (1.4) 11.4 (0.4) .001 .04
  Lead source investigation 25 75.8 (7.6) 15.6 (2.4)
Initial blood lead
concentration
15-19 μg/dL
(N = 876)
205 23.4 (1.4) 12.7(0.8)
Method of contact
  Mail 67 15.9 (1.8) 18.2 (1.8) <.001 <.001
  Telephone 0 NA NA
  Home visit 138 30.4 (2.2) 10.0 (0.6)
Case management service
  Education  184 24.9 (1.6) 13.4 (0.8) .02 .004
  Lead source investigation 21 15.3 (3.1) 6.1 (0.9)

ANOVA indicates analysis of variance; NA, not applicable.
aStates are Iowa, Montana, New Mexico, Ohio, Rhode Island, and Wisconsin.
 

Back to top

 




 



The opinions expressed by authors contributing to this journal do not necessarily reflect the opinions of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, the Public Health Service, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, or the authors’ affiliated institutions. Use of trade names is for identification only and does not imply endorsement by any of the groups named above.


 Home 

Privacy Policy | Accessibility

CDC Home | Search | Health Topics A-Z

This page last reviewed March 30, 2012

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion
 HHS logoUnited States Department of
Health and Human Services